标记理论
- 格式:ppt
- 大小:121.50 KB
- 文档页数:22
标记理论探析1 引言标记理论(markedness theory)是以标记概念为基础,是语言学中用以分析语言系统的一条重要原则。
根据这条原则,对立的语言成分或特征被赋予不同的值,赋予正值即为有标记的(marked),赋予负值或中和值则为无标记的(unmarked)。
标记理论的主要目的是通过分析语言各子系统中的标记现象来建立语言的标记模式(markedness pattern)。
这一理论是二十世纪三十年代由布拉格学派首先提出,距今已经历了近七十年的发展。
其间,生成音系学、语言类型学、生成语法学、功能语法学等从不同的角度将标记理论应用于分析和描写语言中的种种标记现象和特征,从而在不同程度上对标记理论的发展做出了自己的贡献。
本文拟就标记理论及其相关的几个基本问题——标记理论的涵义、标记确定的标准和标记转移现象作一初步探讨。
2 标记理论的涵义标记理论的基本涵义是:在语言中,相对于有标记成分而言无标记成分更为基本,更为自然,更为常用。
然而,在不同的语言学流派中,标记理论的涵义却不尽相同。
(1)根据布拉格学派,在两个对立的语言成分中具有某一(区别性)特征的成分是有标记的,缺少某一(区别性)特征的成分是无标记的。
例如在/t/和/d/这两个对立的音素中,前者是无标记的,因为/t/没有浊音(voicing),而后者则因为有浊音而成为有标记。
再比如英语名词的“数”,复数是有标记的,一般要加-(e)s,而单数为无标记,不加-(e)s。
根据Croft (1990),布拉格学派的标记理论是绝对的二分模式,即某一语言成分要么为有标记,要么为无标记。
(2)在语言类型学中,标记概念是指语言成分(如屈折变化)的非对称现象。
那些普遍性的或多数语言存在的特征为无标记的,而那些为某一语言特有或少数几种语言可见的特征则是有标记的。
类型学中的标记理论是建立在跨语言的比较分析之上,所以标记概念已不再是传统意义上(即布拉格学派的标记理论)的绝对概念,而是一个相对概念。
标记理论的介绍作者:马丽利来源:《活力》2015年第21期[摘要]在任何语言体系中,都存在着对立但不对称的现象。
标记理论便是基于对这一现象的阐述而被广泛应用到语言范畴的各个层面之中。
标记理论的研究不仅有助于我们更全面地了解与掌握个别语言的特征,对语言共性和语言变异的研究都作出了巨大的贡献。
本文论述了标记理论的创立、有无标记项的四大判别标准。
[关键词]不对称;标记理论;判别标准一、标记理论的创立语言中的标记现象是指一个范畴内部存在的某种对立但不对称的现象。
例如,在日语的句法领域中,存在“主动”与“被动”这一组不对称概念,动词原形表示主动态,而被动态要将动词结尾按照相应规则变成“られる”的形式。
我们将动词原形称为“无标记项”,将带有“られる”标记的被动形式称为“有标记项”。
有关这种不对称现象的理论称为“标记理论”。
在二十世纪三十年代初,俄国语言学家特鲁别茨柯依最先提出了标记理论。
并将这一概念中的有标记和无标记的对立运用到了音位学中,他认为在音位学中有三种对立。
第一种是有无对立,也可叫做正负对立,即指甲乙两个音位的唯一区别在于某一特征为甲有而乙无。
例如在日语假名中,[が]与[か]的对立即为有无对立。
二者在发音时,前者有声带振动,而后者无声带振动,除此特征外,二者的其他发音特征完全相同。
第二种是程度对立,即指几个音位之间的对立在于具有某一特征的程度不等。
例如英语音标中[i][e][?]的对立在于开口度的大小不等。
第三种是均等对立,即指相对立的音位各有各的区别性特征。
例如英语中[b][d][g]的对立在于分别为唇印、齿音和腭音。
而布拉格学派的雅科布逊同样也是标记理论的创立者之一。
他与特鲁别茨柯依的观点有所不同,他只主张一种对立,即二分对立,一个成分要么是有标记的,要么是无标记的。
也就是说他并不承认特鲁别茨柯依所说的程度对立和均等对立。
他的贡献是把标记理论的运用从音位学扩展到了形态学。
并且,他认为,在音位学中,有标记和无标记的对立是一种排除关系。
标记理论在国外的研究发展阶段摘要:标记理论是研究语言中对立不对称现象的理论,本文简要介绍了国外标记理论发展的五个阶段及其主要成果。
关键词:标记理论;国外;发展阶段;【中图分类号】g640标记是语言学学科中一个重要概念。
语言中的标记现象是指一个范畴内部存在的某种不对称现象。
研究语言中这种不对称现象的理论就叫标记理论(markedness theory)。
标记理论已成为语言界一个主要的理论思想,是近代语言学研究中常用的一种分析语言的手段,可运用于语言的一切方面。
王立非(1991)在国内第一次描述了标记理论从产生到成熟的发展过程,将其划分为四个阶段:特鲁别茨柯依的创立阶段、雅克布逊的发展阶段、乔姆斯基的改进阶段和当代语言学家的扩展阶段。
本文在王立非划分基础上,丰富增加了后来影响力较大的类型学阶段。
一、特鲁别茨柯依——创立音位标记概念阶段20世纪30年代初,布拉格学派(prague school)的代表人物、俄国语言学家特鲁别茨柯依(n.s.trubetzkoy)最早提出了标记概念。
他在其经典著作《音位学原理》(the principles of phonology)中,探讨了不同种类的音位对立,其中标记概念就来源于音位对立的中和现象。
他观察发现:在一种语言里,有些二元对立项总是处于“正负对立”或是“有无对立”中,即在两个对立的音位中,一方具有某一特征,而另一方没有这一特征。
例如英语中/p/和/b/之间的对立,/b/具有“浊化”特征,而/p/不具有这一特征;另外有些二元对立项却在某些位置上出现中和,即两个音位的对立关系在一定的语言环境中消失。
例如英语中/t/和/d/的对立在某些位置上不存在,如出现在词首/s/音后的只能是/t/,而不是/d/,它们之间的对立属于可中和的对立。
依据特鲁别茨柯依的观点,在“正负对立”的二元对立中,具有该特征的成分称作有标记项,没有该特征成分的称作无标记项;而在“可中和对立”的二元对立项中,出现在中和位置上的成分是无标记项(the unmarked term),不能出现在中和位置上的成分是有标记项(the marked term)。
论标记理论和原型理论的关联摘要:标记理论是语言学中用以分析语言系统的重要原则之一。
标记性体现了语言范畴内部存在的对立的不对称关系,这一概念被广泛应用于音位学、语义学、语法分析和语用学中。
原型范畴理论是认知语言学的主要理论之一。
本文试图用原型范畴理论对词语层面上的标记性现象作出解释,来说明两种理论的关联之处。
关键词:原型理论,标记理论,范畴,有标记,无标记一.标记理论标记理论是结构主义语言学的重要理论之一。
布拉格学派的代表人物之一特鲁别茨柯依在《音位学原理》中提出了9种不同的音位对立,其中对标记概念的产生影响最大的是否定对立。
否定对立中,两个对立项除了具有共同的特征外,一个具有确定的特征(肯定形式的特征),另一个则不具有确定的特征(否定形式的特征)。
具有某种特征的为有标记项(the marked term),没有这种特征的为无标记项(the unmarked term)(刘润清,2002:96)。
雅柯布逊将这一概念引入语法和词汇领域,用来描写语法和语义现象。
此后语言学家又进一步发展了音位的标记理论,他们认为,语言系统中有一部分单位负载的意义是中性的,为无标记项;与其相对应的另一部分单位则在中性意义上附加了某些特征意义,为有标记项”无标记项比有标记项更基本、更自然、更常用,而有标记成分传达的信息比无标记成分所传达得信息更精确、更具体(李玉萍,2004)。
标记概念在语言分析的各个层面上均有解释作用,目前这一概念已广泛应用于语言研究的各个分支。
二.原型理论原型理论(prototype theory)是在现代哲学思想的发展、人类学的调查结果和认知心理学实验研究的基础上产生的。
其产生更多的是得益于上个世纪下半叶人类学与认知心理学对色彩词语以及人类家庭亲属词汇的广泛调查与深入研究。
(梁晓波、李勇忠,2006)与亚理士多德开始的经典理论的传统范畴观不同,认知语言学的原型理论认为许多范畴都是围绕一个原型而构成的,判断某事务是否属于某范畴,不是看它是否具备该范畴成员所有的共同特性,而是看它与其原型之间是否具有足够的维特根斯坦所说的“家族相似性”(family resemblance)。
论篇名语言的标记性∗刘云(北京大学计算语言学研究所 北京 100871)提要 篇名语言与自然语言相比有众多差异,本文从标记理论出发给予了统一的解释,指出篇名语言与自然语言相比是一种标记性语言。
文章揭示了篇名语言标记性的种种表现形式,并分析了篇名语言标记性的语用动因,具体包括三个因素:称名性、话题性、经济性。
关键词 篇名语言 标记性 语用动因零、标记理论语言中的标记现象(markedness)是指一个范畴内部存在的某种不对称现象,例如就英语名词的“数”这一语法范畴而言,复数形式是有标记的(marked),一般要加-s标记,如teachers等,单数形式则是无标记的(un-marked),不需加-s。
概括地说,在“数”这一范畴内,复数是有标记项,单数是无标记项。
有标记和无标记的对立是语言里的普遍现象。
沈家煊(1997)指出,有标记和无标记的对立在语言分析的所有层次上都起作用。
人们大多关注语音(如英语“/b/”与“/p/”在有无[带声]上的对立)、形态(如英语“woman”与“man”的对立)、句法(“现在时”和“过去时”的对立,“肯定句”和“否定句”的对立)、语义(如反义形容词“大”和“小”、“深”和“浅”的对立)、语用(如英语初次相识时道声“How do you do”是无标记的,保持沉默则是有标记的),等等。
本文关注的是篇名语句针对自然语句的标记性,认为篇名语句是有标记的,自然语句是无标记的。
因此,本文所称的标记性是从篇章的角度而言的。
一、篇名语言与自然语言的异同篇名语言与自然语言有许多共同之处,自然语言中的词、短语、单句和复句都可以毫无变化地充当篇名,如:(1)狐狸(《当代小说》1998年8期)(2)不归的水手(《北方文学》1993年9期)(3)林业是中国环境资源立法最早最多最快的部门(《中国绿色时报》2002年10月9日)(4)课文是什么?语文教什么?(《福建中学教学》1999年1期)例(1)是词,例(2)是短语,例(3)是单句(只不过没有使用句号),例(4)是复句,这些篇名都可以直接用在自然语句中。
MarkednessFrom Wikipedia, the free encyclopediaJump to: navigation, search"Unmarked" redirects here. For undecorated law enforcement vehicles, see Police car#Functional types.Markedness, a term which originated in linguistics, is the state of standing out as unusual or difficult in comparison to a more common or regular form. In a marked/unmarked relation, one term of an opposition is the broader, dominant one. The dominant default or minimum effort form is known as the 'unmarked' term and the other, secondary one is the 'marked' term. In other words, it is the characterization of a "normal" linguistic unit (i.e. the unmarked term) compared to the unit's possible "irregular" forms (i.e. the marked term). For example, in English, irregular verbs are marked, while regular verbs are unmarked in comparison.In linguistics, markedness can apply, among others, phonological, grammatical, and semantic oppositions, defining them in terms of 'marked' and 'unmarked' oppositions like honest(unmarked) vs. dishonest(marked). Marking may be purely semantic, or may be realized as extra morphology. The term derives from the 'marking' of a grammatical role with a suffix or other element, but has been extended to situations where there is no morphological distinction.In the social sciences more broadly, markedness is used to distinguish two meanings of the same term, where one is common usage (unmarked sense) and the other is specialized to a cultural context (marked sense).Contents[hide]∙ 1 Marked and unmarked word pairs∙ 2 Background in the Prague School∙ 3 Jakobsonian tradition∙ 4 Cultural markedness∙ 5 Local markedness and markedness reversals∙ 6 Universals and frequency∙7 Diagnostics∙8 Markedness in generative grammar∙9 See also∙10 References∙11 BibliographyMarked and unmarked word pairs[edit]In terms of lexical opposites, marked form is a non-basic, often one with inflectional or derivational endings. Thus, a morphologically negative word form is marked as opposed to a positive one: happy/unhappy, honest/dishonest, fair/unfair, clean/unclean and so forth. Similarly, unaffixed masculine or singular forms are taken to be unmarked in contrast to affixed feminine or plural forms: lion/lioness, host/hostess, automobile/automobiles, child/children. An unmarked form is also a default form. For example, the unmarked lion can refer to a male or female, while lioness is marked because it can refer only to females.The default nature allows unmarked lexical forms to be identified even when the opposites are not morphologically related. In the pairs old/young, big/little, happy/sad, clean/dirty, the first term of each pair is taken as unmarked because it occurs generally in questions. For example, English speakers typically ask how old (big, happy, clean) … something or someone is. To use the marked term presupposes youth, smallness, unhappiness, or dirtiness.Background in the Prague School[edit]While the idea of linguistic asymmetry predated the actual coining of the terms 'marked' and 'unmarked' the modern concept of markedness originated in the Prague School structuralism of Roman Jakobson and Nikolai Trubetzkoy as a means of characterizing binary oppositions.[1]Both sound and meaning were analyzed into systems of binary distinctive features. Edwin Battistella said "Binarism suggests symmetry and equivalence in linguistic analysis; markedness adds the idea of hierarchy."[2]Trubetzkoy and Jakobson analyzed phonological oppositions such as nasal versus non-nasal as defined as the presence versus the absence of nasality; the presence of the feature, nasality, was marked; its absence, non-nasality, was unmarked. For Jakobson and Trubetzkoy, binary phonological features formed part of a universal feature alphabet applicable to all languages. In his 1932 article "Structure of the Russian Verb", Jakobson extended the concept to grammatical meanings in which the marked element 'announces the existence of [some meaning] A' while the unmarked element 'does not announce the existence of A, i.e., does not state whether A is present or not'.[3] Forty years later, Jakobson described language by saying that "every single constituent of a linguistic system is built on an oppositionof two logical contradictories: the presence of an attribute('markedness') in contraposition to its absence ('unmarkedness')."[4]In his 1941 Child Language, Aphasia, and Universals of Language, Jakobson also suggested that phonological markedness played a role in language acquisition and loss. Drawing on existing studies of acquisition and aphasia, Jakobson suggested a mirror image relationship determined by a universal feature hierarchy of marked and unmarked oppositions. Today many still see Jakobson's theory of phonological acquisition as identifying useful tendencies.[5]Jakobsonian tradition[edit]The work of Cornelius van Schooneveld, Edna Andrews, Rodney Sangster, Yishai Tobin and others on 'semantic invariance' (different general meanings reflected in the contextual specific meanings of features) has further developed the semantic analysis of grammatical items in terms of marked and unmarked features. Other semiotically-oriented work has investigated the isomorphism of form and meaning with less emphasis on invariance, including the efforts of Henning Andersen, Michael Shapiro, and Edwin Battistella. Shapiro and Andrews have especially made connections between the semiotic of C. S. Peirce and markedness, treating it as "as species of interpretant" in Peirce's sign-object-interpretant triad.Functional linguists such as Thomas Givon have suggested that markedness is related to cognitive complexity—"in terms of attention, mental effort or processing time".[6] And linguistic 'naturalists' view markedness relations in terms of the ways in which extralinguistic principles of perceptibility and psychological efficiency determine what is natural in language. Linguist Willi Mayerthaler, for example, defines unmarked categories as those "in agreement with the typical attributes of the speaker".[7]Cultural markedness[edit]Since a main component of markedness is the information content and information value of an element,[8] some studies have taken markedness as an encoding of that which is unusual or informative, and this is reflected in formal probabilistic definitions of Markedness and Informedness (as chance-correct unidirectional components of the Matthews Correlation Coefficient corresponding to deltap and deltap'.[9]Conceptual familiarity with cultural norms provided by familiar categories creates a groundagainst which marked categories provide a figure, opening the way for markedness to be applied to cultural and social categorization.As early as the 1930s Jakobson had already suggested applying markedness to all oppositions, explicitly mentioning such pairs as life/death, liberty/bondage, sin/virtue, and holiday/working day. Linda Waugh extended this to oppositions like male/female, white/black,sighted/blind, hearing/deaf, heterosexual/homosexual, right/left, fertility/barrenness, clothed/nude, and spoken language/written language.[10] Battistella expanded this with the demonstration of how cultures align markedness values to create cohesive symbol systems, illustrating with examples based on Joseph Needham's work.[11] Other work has applied markedness to stylistics, music, myth.[12]Local markedness and markedness reversals[edit]Markedness depends on context. What is more marked in some general contexts may be less marked in other local contexts. Thus, "ant" is less marked than "ants" on the morphological level, but on the semantic (and frequency) levels it may be more marked since ants are more often encountered many at once than one at a time. Often a more general markedness relation may be reversed in a particular context. Thus, voicelessness of consonants is typically unmarked. But between vowels or in the neighborhood of voiced consonants, voicing may be the expected or unmarked value.Reversal is reflected in certain Frisian words' plural and singular forms:[13]In Frisian, nouns with irregular singular-plural stem variations are undergoing regularization. Usually this means that the plural is reformed to be a regular form of the singular:∙Old paradigm: "koal" (coal), "kwallen" (coals) → regularized forms: "Koal" (coal), "Koalen" (coals).However, a number of words instead reform the singular by extending the form of the plural:∙Old paradigm: "earm" (arm), "jermen" (arms) → regularized forms: "jerm" (arm), "jermen" (arms)The common feature of the nouns that regularize the singular to match the plural is that they occur more often in pairs or groups than singly; they are said to be semantically (but not morphologically) locally unmarked in the plural.Universals and frequency[edit]Joseph Greenberg's 1966 book Language Universals was an influential application of markedness to typological linguistics and a break from the tradition of Jakobson and Trubetzkoy. Greenberg took frequency to be the primary determining factor of markedness in grammar and suggested that unmarked categories could be determined by "the frequency of association of things in the real world".Greenberg also applied frequency cross-linguistically, suggesting that unmarked categories would be those that are unmarked in a wide number of languages. However, critics have argued that frequency is problematic because categories that are cross-linguistically infrequent may have a high distribution in a particular language.[14]More recently the insights related to frequency have been formalized as chance-corrected conditional probabilities, with Informedness (deltap') and Markedness (deltap) corresponding to the different directions of prediction in human association research (binary associations or distinctions) [15] and more generally (including features with more than two distinctions).[9]Universals have also been connected to implicational laws. This entails that a category is taken as marked if every language that has the marked category also has the unmarked one but not vice versa.Diagnostics[edit]Markedness has been extended and reshaped over the past century and reflects a range of loosely connected theoretical approaches. From emerging in the analysis of binary oppositions, it has become a global semiotic principle, a means of encoding naturalness and language universals, and a terminology for studying defaults and preferences in language acquisition. What connects various approaches is a concern for the evaluation of linguistic structure, though the details of how markedness is determined and what its implications and diagnostics are varies widely. Other approaches to universal markedness relations focus on functional economic and iconic motivations, tying recurring symmetries to properties of communication channels and communication events. Croft (1990), for example, notes that asymmetries among linguistic elements may be explainable in terms economy of form, in terms of iconism between the structure of language and conceptualization of the world.Markedness in generative grammar[edit]Markedness entered generative linguistic theory through Chomsky and Halle's The Sound Pattern of English. For Chomsky and Halle, phonological features went beyond a universal phonetic vocabulary to encompass an 'evaluation metric', a means of selecting the most highly-valued adequate grammar. In The Sound Pattern of English, the value of a grammar was the inverse of the number of features required in that grammar. However, Chomsky and Halle realized that their initial approach to phonological features made implausible rules and segment inventories as highly valued as natural ones. The unmarked value of a feature was cost-free with respect to the evaluation metric, while the marked feature values were counted by the metric. Segment inventories could also be evaluated according to the number of marked features. However, the use of phonological markedness as part of the evaluation metric was never able to fully account for the fact that some features are more likely than others or for the fact that phonological systems must have a certain minimal complexity and symmetry[16]In generative syntax, markedness as feature-evaluation did not receive the same attention that it did in phonology. Chomsky came to view unmarked properties as an innate preference structure based first in constraints and later in parameters of universal grammar. In their 1977 article 'Filters and Control', Chomsky and Howard Lasnik extended this to view markedness as part of a theory of 'core grammar':We will assume that [Universal Grammar] in not an 'undifferentiated' system, but rather incorporates something analogous to a 'theory of markedness' Specifically, there is a theory of core grammar with highly restricted options, limited expressive power, and a few parameters. Systems that fall within core grammar constitute 'the unmarked case';we may think of them as optimal in terms of the evaluation metric. An actual language is determined by fixing the parameters of core grammar and then adding rules or conditions, using much richer resources, ... These added properties of grammars we may think of as the syntactic analogue of irregular verbs.[17]A few years later Chomsky would describe it this way:The distinction between core and periphery leaves us with three notions of markedness: core versus periphery, internal to the core, and internal to the periphery. The second has to do with the way parameters are set in the absence of evidence. As for the third, there are, no doubt, significant regularities even in departures from the core principles (for example, in irregular verb morphology in English), and it may be that peripheral constructions are related to the core in systematic ways, say by relaxing certain conditions of core grammar.[18]Some generative researchers have applied markedness to second-language acquisition theory, treating it as an inherent learning hierarchy which reflects the sequence in which constructions are acquired, the difficulty of acquiring certain constructions, and the transferability of rules across languages [REF NEEDED]. More recently, Optimality Theory approaches emerging in the 1990s have incorporated markedness in the ranking of constraints.[19]See also[edit]∙Lemma (morphology)∙Lexeme∙Inflection∙Matthews correlation coefficient∙Marker (linguistics)∙Null morpheme∙Underlying representationReferences[edit]1.Jump up ^ Andersen Henning "Markedness--The First 150 Years". In Markedness inSynchrony and Diachrony. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, 19892.Jump up ^ Battistella, Edwin Markedness: The Evaluative Superstructure of Language.SUNY Press, 1990.3.Jump up ^ Jakobson, R 1932 "The Structure of the Russian Verb" reprinted in Russianand Slavic Grammar Studies, 1931-1981,Mouton,1984.4.Jump up ^ Jakobson, R Verbal Communication. Scientific American 227: 72-80, 19725.Jump up ^ Battistella, Edwin, The Logic of Markedness. NY: Oxford Uniuversity Press,1996,6.Jump up ^ Givôn Thomas Syntax: A Functionai-Typological Introduction,volume2,John Benjamins, Amsterdam 1990.7.Jump up ^ Mayerthaler Willi Morphological Naturalness. Karoma, Ann Arbor,1988.8.Jump up ^ Battistella, Markedness, 1990.9.^ Jump up to: a b Powers, David M W (2007/2011). "Evaluation: From Precision,Recall and F-Score to ROC, Informedness, Markedness & Correlation". Journal ofMachine Learning Technologies2 (1): 37–63.10.Jump up ^ Waugh, Linda Waugh, Marked and Unmarked: A Choice BetweenUnequals in Semiotic Structure. Semiotica 38: 299-318, 198211.Jump up ^ Battistella, Edwin, Markedness, 1990, 188-189.12.Jump up ^ Myers-Scotton Carol (ed.) Codes and Consequences: Choosing LinguisticVarieties. Oxford, 1998; Hatten Robert Musical Meaning in Beethoven: Markedness,Correlation, and Interpretation. Indiana University Press, 1994; Liszka, James J. TheSemiotic of Myth. Indiana University Press, 198913.Jump up ^ Tiersma, Peter. "Local and General Markedness", Language, 1982.14.Jump up ^ Battistella, Edwin, The Logic of Markedness, 1996, 51.15.Jump up ^ Perruchet, P.; Peereman, R. (2004). "The exploitation of distributionalinformation in syllable processing". J. Neurolinguistics17: 97−119.16.Jump up ^ Kean, Mary-Louise, The Theory of Markedness in Generative GrammarIndiana University Linguistics Club, Bloomington, IN, 198017.Jump up ^ Chomsky Noam and Howard Lasnik, Filters and Control. Linguistic Inquiry8.3: 425-504 197718.Jump up ^ Chomsky, Noam. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use.Praeger,198619.Jump up ^ see Archangeli 1997Bibliography[edit]∙Andersen Henning 1989 "Markedness--The First 150 Years", In Markedness in Synchrony and Diachrony. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin.∙Andrews Edna 1990 Markedness Theory: The Union of Asymmetry and Semiosis in Language, Duke University Press, Durham,NC.∙Archangeli Diana 1997 "Optimality Theory: An Introduction to Linguistics in the 1990s", In Optimality Theory: An Overview. Blackwell, Maiden, MA.∙Battistella Edwin 1990 Markedness: The Evaluative Superstructure of Language, SUNY Press, Albany, NY.∙Battistella Edwin 1996 The Logic of Markedness, Oxford University Press, NY.∙Chandler, Daniel 2002/2007 Semiotics: The Basics, Routledge, London, UK.∙Chandler, Daniel 2005 Entry on markedness. In John Protevi (Ed.) (2005) Edinburgh Dictionary of Continental Philosophy, Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.∙Chomsky, Noam and Morris Halle 1968 The Sound Pattern of English, Harper and Row, NY∙Greenberg Joseph Language Universals, Mouton, The Hague, 1966.∙Trask, R.L. 1999 Key Concepts in Language and Linguistics, London and New York: Routledge.。